Date: November 18, 2015
Source:
University of Louisville (Which they publish after the World Series,
and University of Louisville, as in “Louisville Slugger” did the
research-who by the way is now owned by a European company)
Summary:
Researchers
have increased our understanding of how people and animals deal with
sensorimotor delay in day-to-day interactions by analyzing the hunting
skills of salamanders.
If
a baseball player waits until he sees the ball arrive in front of him
to swing his bat, he will miss miserably. By the time the batter sees
the ball's position, plans his swing and moves the bat, the ball will be
firmly in the catcher's mitt.
This
time lag is known as sensorimotor delay. University of Louisville
researcher Bart Borghuis, Ph.D., has increased our understanding of how
people and animals deal with this delay in day-to-day interactions by
analyzing the hunting skills of salamanders. His article, "The Role of
Motion Extrapolation in Amphibian Prey Capture," is published in today's
issue of The Journal of Neuroscience.
A
skilled baseball player compensates for sensorimotor delay by
predicting when the ball will cross the plate and starting his swing in
time to meet it. Borghuis' research reveals the salamander also predicts
the future location of its prey as it catches moving fruit flies by
projecting its long, sticky tongue.
The
sensorimotor delay is caused by the time it takes for the visual image
to be processed by the retina, time to plan the motor action and time to
activate the motion. When a salamander hopes to catch a moving fly, in
the time it takes to make the strike -- about 230 milliseconds -- the
fly will have moved from the location it was in when the salamander
launched its attack. If the salamander sends its tongue to the location
where it sees the fly, by the time the tongue gets there, the fly will
be gone. Despite this delay, salamanders are efficient hunters, catching
their prey more than 90 percent of the time in Borghuis' experiments.
Why are salamanders so effective in their attacks?
Borghuis,
assistant professor in the Department of Anatomical Sciences and
Neurobiology at UofL, and Anthony Leonardo, Ph.D., of the Howard Hughes
Medical Institute, used high speed videography to capture 270 instances
of salamanders striking at flies. Through analysis of the videos,
Borghuis developed an algorithm that predicted where the salamander's
tongue would strike based on the fly's path.
The
algorithm mimics the salamanders' process using extrapolation to
anticipate the prey's position in the future based on its bearing and
velocity. The salamanders' tongue strikes were consistent with the
algorithm, and were consistently successful -- unless the fly changed
course between the time the salamander initiated the attack and the time
of the actual strike.
In
successful strikes, the salamander caught the fly by sending its tongue
tip to the position where the fly was when the tongue arrived. When the
salamanders missed, the salamander's tongue struck the location where
the fly would have been had it continued on the same path it had been
following. However, in these cases, the fly had changed direction after
the salamander launched its attack.
"The
misses confirmed the model," Borghuis said. "This is the first
demonstration that the salamanders were making a prediction."
The
tongue struck where the fly never had been, yet would have been had the
fly continued its previous course of motion. Thus the salamander was
predicting where the fly would be at the time the tongue reached it
based on the fly's direction and speed.
"This
information adds to a small set of clear examples of how vertebrates --
including humans -- use prediction for dealing with delays in motor
processing," Borghuis said. "Now that we know how the salamander does
this, we can further investigate the neuromechanisms that make this
happen."
Story Source:
The above post is reprinted from materials provided by University of Louisville. Note: Materials may be edited for content and length.
Journal Reference:
- Bart G. Borghuis and Anthony Leonardo. The Role of Motion Extrapolation in Amphibian Prey Capture. Journal of Neuroscience, 2015 DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3189-15.2015
Cite This Page:
University of Louisville. "What salamanders can teach us about baseball." ScienceDaily. ScienceDaily, 18 November 2015. <www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2015/11/151118160138.htm>.
No comments:
Post a Comment
You only need to enter your comment once! Comments will appear once they have been moderated. This is so as to stop the would-be comedian who has been spamming the comments here with inane and often offensive remarks. You know who you are!