New York Times, Science Take,
10/24/17 by James Gorman and Cristopeher Whitworh
South American poison frogs are
known to most people for their poison — and their brilliant colors. But in
almost all species, the poison frog carries tadpoles on its back to a rain
forest pool for their final growth stage.
One species, Allobates femoralis, may or may not be mildly poisonous, and for color
it has a splash of orange on its thighs. Thus its endearing common name, the
brilliant-thighed poison frog. And sure enough, the females lay the eggs in a
relatively dry spot in the rain forest, and once the embryos reach tadpole
stage, the males load them on their backs and take off.
Andrius Pasukonis, a researcher
associated with the University of Vienna and Harvard University, has been
studying these frogs in the rain forest of French Guiana to learn how the males
find the pools and what triggers this complicated act of tadpole transport. He
and Kristina Beck, a graduate student at the University of Vienna, and others,
determined in one project that the frogs don’t search for pools but return
directly to pools they know.
“They really rely on their
spatial memory; they don’t randomly wander around the forest until they bump
into pools,” Dr. Pasukonis said. The publication of that research was the first
time the navigational
behavior of the frogs was described, he said.
He and his colleagues also found
that they could trigger
the complex behavior of tadpole transport simply by putting tadpoles
on a frog’s back. The tadpoles could come from any frog at all. And the method
worked just as well with females as males. That was a surprise, because in
nature females only carry tadpoles if the male has disappeared, and then they
will only carry tadpoles from the exact spot where they laid their eggs. Males
will carry any tadpoles found in their territory.
“It caught us off guard,” Dr.
Pasukonis said, that behavior which seemed that it might require several
actions to trigger it, such as breeding and the presence of eggs or tadpoles in
the right place, could be initiated by one simple event.
“Once the tadpoles are on the
back, the rule is to go transport them,” Dr. Pasukonis said.
“I’m still surprised that it
actually works.” But, he said, the signal that tells the frogs to move is clear
and simple.
“Tadpoles on the back — you have
to go.”
Dr. Pasukonis and Ms. Beck worked
on the two projects with Eva Ringler and Max Ringler, both of the University of
Vienna, using a small river island with both natural pools and man made pools.
They lived in huts at a French research station, which had unexpected benefits.
“We get frozen food,” Dr.
Pasukonis said, “but it will include five types of cheese as well, so you feel
that you’re in France.”
Tracking inch-long frogs up to a
couple of hundred yards in dense vegetation, sometimes over a day or more, was
not an easy task. They first had to catch frogs, and then equip them with
transponders attached to tiny silicon waistbands.
The finding that tadpole
transport was a fully developed behavior in both males and females, even though
males did it almost exclusively in nature, was another significant part of the
research, said Dr. Pasukonis.
He said the study of complex
behavior in amphibians is in its infancy, and hopes to perhaps delve into the underlying
neuroscience.
No comments:
Post a Comment
You only need to enter your comment once! Comments will appear once they have been moderated. This is so as to stop the would-be comedian who has been spamming the comments here with inane and often offensive remarks. You know who you are!