Science
News by Susan Milius, Science News 9/15/16
Humankind
is loud, and research already suggests that birds alter their singing in urban
noise. Now tests show that bats listening for the frogs they hunt switch from
mostly quiet eavesdropping to pinging echolocating when artificial sounds mask
the frog calls. That way, the bats can detect the motion of the frogs’ vocal
sac poofing out with each call, researchers report in the Sept. 16 Science.
That
switch in sensory tactics could make bats the only animals besides people shown
to react this way to interfering din in the classic cocktail party scenario,
says study coauthor Wouter Halfwerk of Vrije University Amsterdam. People
straining to hear each other over the cacophony of a party can get a boost in
communication by paying attention to each other’s mouth movements. He points
out that watching someone’s lips lets people tolerate about an extra 20
decibels of tipsy shrieking and shouting.
Conservation
biologists worry about the effects of human racket on other residents of the
planet. Other researchers, for instance, found that noise interfered with
pallid bats’ success in hunting insects on the wing. The fringe-lipped bats (Trachops cirrhosus) in the new study,
however, specialize in frogs instead of insects. Hungry bats listen to frog
choruses and swoop out of the darkness to carry off a male chirping his
advertisements for a mate. “A talking pickle” is what Halfwerk calls the frog.
Researchers
tested 12 wild-caught bats in outdoor flight cages in Panama. Bats perching
(upside down, of course) in cages were perfectly willing to make a grab at
robotic frogs deployed in the cages. The robofrogs, modeled by an artist on the
túngara species the bats naturally hunt, sit motionless but can on command
start inflating a specially constructed balloon in time with broadcast calls.
In this
setup, interfering noise changed normal hunting. When researchers broadcast
sounds that partially masked the main frequency of telltale frog calls, bats
waited longer than normal to strike and also strongly preferred pouncing on a
robofrog that was inflating his sac instead of an identical frog squatting
nearby with a deflated sac. Recordings of bat noises from the perch showed that
the hunters were pinging fast echolocation sounds instead of mostly listening
for the pickle to betray its location.
Even with
the strategy switch, the bats aren’t completely making up for the noise
nuisance, Jinhong Luo at Johns Hopkins University points out. A sensory
biologist, he has tested noise effects on other bats but was not involved in
this project. Looking at the new data, he notes that frog-eating bats in
echolocating mode are slower to leave their perches and swoop than bats in
eavesdropping mode. He also cautions about generalizing to the other 1,300-plus
bat species. Many of them are already using echolocation to hunt insects and
may not have a backup prey-finder method when noise complicates their
foraging.
Citations
D.G.E.
Gomes et al. Bats perceptually weight prey cues across sensory systems when
hunting in noise. Science. Vol. 353, September 16, 2013, p. 1277. doi:
101126/science.aaf7934.
Further
Reading
J.P.
Bunkley and J.R. Barber. Noise Reduces Foraging Efficiency in Pallid Bats
(Antrozous pallidus). Ethology. November 2015, p. 1116. doi: 10.1111/eth.12428.
W.
Halfwerk et al. Negative impact of traffic noise on avian reproductive success.
Journal of Applied Ecology. Vol. 48, February 2011, p. 210. doi:
10.1111/j.1365-2664.2010.01914x.
S.
Milius. A coast-to-coast picture of America’s cacophony of sounds. Science
News. Vol. 187, February 21, 2015, p. 32.
S.
Milius. Noise made by humans can be bad news for animals. Science News. Vol.
187, February 21, 2015, p. 22.
No comments:
Post a Comment
You only need to enter your comment once! Comments will appear once they have been moderated. This is so as to stop the would-be comedian who has been spamming the comments here with inane and often offensive remarks. You know who you are!