When a meerkat hears the alarm call, it drops its food and runs for cover thinking there's a predator around, allowing the drongo to get a free lunch.
Fork tailed drongo (Dicrurus adsimilis)
This is the first time scientists have shown that birds use alarm calls specifically to deceive other species.
'Meerkats have learnt to recognise the alarm calls made by different bird species and flee for cover when they hear them,' says Tom Flower from the University of Cambridge and author of the study, which is published in Proceedings of the Royal Society B.
This latest study suggests that fork-tailed drongos (Dicrurus adsimilis) have exploited the meerkats, using both their own and mimicked alarm calls to scare meerkats when they see them with scorpions or geckos.
Flower was working on meerkats in the Kalahari in southern Africa, when he noticed drongos following them around. He noticed the birds making alarm calls when there were no predators nearby and thought they must be doing it on purpose.
'Drongos are following meerkats and pied-babblers around in the hope of catching their food,' says Flower.
He thought the birds might moderate the number of times they lied to avoid blowing their cover. But this doesn't seem to be the case. 'They just turn up and steal as much food as they can get away with,' Flower says.
The researchers think the birds use a repertoire of alarm calls to keep meerkats on their toes, and to stop the creatures getting suspicious.
'It's like the boy who cried wolf: if drongos use the same call each and every time, meerkats will learn to ignore them, so instead they use different calls,' says Flower.
'Meerkats have to sit up and take note, because they've got too much to lose if they ignore the calls. They could lose their lives,' he adds
Drongos act as sentries for other animals - when they make alarm calls, meerkats and birds called pied-babblers know to run for cover. But if they do it too often, they won't be believed. So, they have to change which alarm call they use to stop meerkats ignoring them.
While scientists know that birds use their own and other birds' alarm calls to alert others to danger, until now, they weren't sure if the calls were used to deceive.
To show that fork-tailed drongos use deceptive alarm calls to get a free lunch, Flower's research had to satisfy three conditions. He had to show that alarm calls are used only in a deceptive context rather than an aggressive context; that true alarm calls made by drongos and other species sound the same as false alarm calls; and that meerkats and pied-babblers run for cover in response to recordings of both false and true alarm calls.
Researchers have suggested a number of possible functions for mimicry, such as that being able to copy other birds might help attract a mate, or that it lets animals communicate with other species. But no-one has shown that having a wide repertoire of calls attracts more females.
Flower's research shows a clear function for vocal mimicry, when before scientists could only speculate. He thinks mimicry is likely to be a fairly simple mechanism, and that drongos work out which calls are most effective by trial and error.
He's now keen to go back to the Kalahari to see how drongos learn to vary their calls.
by Tamera Jones
http://planetearth.nerc.ac.uk/news/story.aspx?id=856
This is the first time scientists have shown that birds use alarm calls specifically to deceive other species.
'Meerkats have learnt to recognise the alarm calls made by different bird species and flee for cover when they hear them,' says Tom Flower from the University of Cambridge and author of the study, which is published in Proceedings of the Royal Society B.
This latest study suggests that fork-tailed drongos (Dicrurus adsimilis) have exploited the meerkats, using both their own and mimicked alarm calls to scare meerkats when they see them with scorpions or geckos.
Flower was working on meerkats in the Kalahari in southern Africa, when he noticed drongos following them around. He noticed the birds making alarm calls when there were no predators nearby and thought they must be doing it on purpose.
'Drongos are following meerkats and pied-babblers around in the hope of catching their food,' says Flower.
He thought the birds might moderate the number of times they lied to avoid blowing their cover. But this doesn't seem to be the case. 'They just turn up and steal as much food as they can get away with,' Flower says.
The researchers think the birds use a repertoire of alarm calls to keep meerkats on their toes, and to stop the creatures getting suspicious.
'It's like the boy who cried wolf: if drongos use the same call each and every time, meerkats will learn to ignore them, so instead they use different calls,' says Flower.
'Meerkats have to sit up and take note, because they've got too much to lose if they ignore the calls. They could lose their lives,' he adds
Drongos act as sentries for other animals - when they make alarm calls, meerkats and birds called pied-babblers know to run for cover. But if they do it too often, they won't be believed. So, they have to change which alarm call they use to stop meerkats ignoring them.
While scientists know that birds use their own and other birds' alarm calls to alert others to danger, until now, they weren't sure if the calls were used to deceive.
To show that fork-tailed drongos use deceptive alarm calls to get a free lunch, Flower's research had to satisfy three conditions. He had to show that alarm calls are used only in a deceptive context rather than an aggressive context; that true alarm calls made by drongos and other species sound the same as false alarm calls; and that meerkats and pied-babblers run for cover in response to recordings of both false and true alarm calls.
Researchers have suggested a number of possible functions for mimicry, such as that being able to copy other birds might help attract a mate, or that it lets animals communicate with other species. But no-one has shown that having a wide repertoire of calls attracts more females.
Flower's research shows a clear function for vocal mimicry, when before scientists could only speculate. He thinks mimicry is likely to be a fairly simple mechanism, and that drongos work out which calls are most effective by trial and error.
He's now keen to go back to the Kalahari to see how drongos learn to vary their calls.
by Tamera Jones
http://planetearth.nerc.ac.uk/news/story.aspx?id=856
No comments:
Post a Comment
You only need to enter your comment once! Comments will appear once they have been moderated. This is so as to stop the would-be comedian who has been spamming the comments here with inane and often offensive remarks. You know who you are!