Unlike many
species, this common reptile migrates from memory
By Timothy Roth,
Aaron R.
Krochmal from Blogs Scientific American January 30, 2019
Animal
migration is one of the most charismatic, awe-inspiring phenomena of the
natural world. Faced with unfavorable, often seasonal, changes in climate or
habitat quality, animals, from birds to butterflies and wildebeests to sea
turtles, migrate vast distances—often thousands of miles—in search of more
favorable conditions. They return the following year, in many cases to the
exact location where their journey began.
We know that
some species use the Earth’s magnetic field to navigate with GPS-like precision
and others gaze skyward and guide their migration by the stars. We are just now
learning that for some animals, the key to a successful migration is all in
their heads—complex thought and memory are also necessary for a successful
migration.
For nearly a
decade, we have studied the roles of cognition, learning and memory in the
migration of a fairly atypical species—the painted turtle, common to
neighborhood ponds and roadside ditches. Yep, that turtle—the one you see in
virtually every small body of water in the Eastern U.S., as well as
occasionally crawling through your backyard or crossing the road by the
grocery.
They’re not
as charismatic as sea turtles, and they sure don’t travel as far (several kilometers
for a painted turtle versus several thousand kilometers for a sea turtle), but
what they lack in outward charisma they make up in tenacity. When these turtles
take to land each summer, migrating to new habitats when their home ponds dry
up, they face seemingly insurmountable odds: scorching heat, dehydration, and
the crushing tug of gravity (you can’t just float around anymore). And that’s
not to mention the new predatory threats from both land and air, and the
ever-present threat of vehicles when crossing a road. For a painted turtle,
there’s a lot going on during migration.
Painted
turtles at our research site, which is on conservation land within a patchwork
of old growth woodlands and agricultural fields, follow long, intricate routes
with amazing precision—specific to within a few meters—to far-off, permanent
water sources year after year, returning home again when the seasons next
change. Is this behavior instinctual or learned?
To see just
where this incredible migratory behavior came from, we introduced into our site
animals without any experience migrating there and monitored their ability to
respond to seasonal changes in their habitats. Would they be able to migrate
successfully?
For some,
yes. Naive juveniles under four years old learned to navigate the complex paths
just as precisely as experienced local turtles and were able to locate far-off
water sources. Naive adults could not. These results suggest a narrow age
window, or critical learning period, in which animals must learn to navigate.
This phenomenon is not unlike the process of language learning in humans.
But how can
they do this? How can a turtle possibly think its way through migration?
They do it
the same way that we do: they
form and remember memories of space and place using the
neurotransmitter acetylcholine, just like humans.
Acetylcholine
has long been known to play a role in spatial memory in mammals. To test
whether the turtle brain also works this way, we gave freely migrating turtles
(both experienced adults and naive juveniles) mind-altering drugs that
temporarily block acetylcholine in their brains.
While the
drugs were active and the turtles were without access to spatial memory, the
adults with previous experience in the system wandered aimlessly and were
unable to follow their traditional migration routes. As soon as the drugs wore
off, they got right back on track and were able to successfully migrate to
their winter home.
And what
about naive juvenile turtles? They
were unaffected by the drug; they followed the paths
perfectly whether on the drug or not. Why? They had no memory to disrupt! This
demonstrates that adults use spatial memory to navigate during migration, and
that they form these memories as juveniles prior to the age of four. This type
of higher-order cognitive processing during migration has been previously
attributed only to birds and mammals.
For some,
yes. Naive juveniles under four years old learned to navigate the complex paths
just as precisely as experienced local turtles and were able to locate far-off
water sources. Naive adults could not. These results suggest a narrow age
window, or critical learning period, in which animals must learn to navigate.
This phenomenon is not unlike the process of language learning in humans.
They do it
the same way that we do: they
form and remember memories of space and place using the
neurotransmitter acetylcholine, just like humans.
Acetylcholine
has long been known to play a role in spatial memory in mammals. To test
whether the turtle brain also works this way, we gave freely migrating turtles
(both experienced adults and naive juveniles) mind-altering drugs that
temporarily block acetylcholine in their brains.
While the
drugs were active and the turtles were without access to spatial memory, the adults
with previous experience in the system wandered aimlessly and were unable to
follow their traditional migration routes. As soon as the drugs wore off, they
got right back on track and were able to successfully migrate to their winter
home.
And
what about naive juvenile turtles? They
were unaffected by the drug; they followed the paths
perfectly whether on the drug or not. Why? They had no memory to disrupt! This
demonstrates that adults use spatial memory to navigate during migration, and
that they form these memories as juveniles prior to the age of four. This type
of higher-order cognitive processing during migration has been previously
attributed only to birds and mammals.
So, it turns
out that if you’re a turtle, migration is more than just showing up and
muddling through on instinct; you actually have to pay attention and think. And
for the turtles, that’s a good thing.
Challenging
environments—those with the most extreme, highly variable conditions—tend to
produce animals with advanced cognitive abilities, including flexibility in
learning and memory.
As climate
change continues to disrupt environments, animals will have to rely on
cognition to learn new things, including altering the timing, direction and
destination of their migrations.
So even
though the odds are stacked against them, maybe, just maybe, turtles
can outsmart the impacts of climate change.
Go to
For authors
background-background-see bottom of page
The views
expressed here are not necessarily those of Scientific American
No comments:
Post a Comment
You only need to enter your comment once! Comments will appear once they have been moderated. This is so as to stop the would-be comedian who has been spamming the comments here with inane and often offensive remarks. You know who you are!